Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation
Contributed by Michael I. Posner, August 16, 2007 (received for review July 26, 2007)
19/05/2011 14:20 (GMT+7) Font size:
|
Recent studies suggest that months to
years of intensive and systematic meditation training can improve
attention. However,
the lengthy training required has made it difficult
to use random assignment of participants to conditions to confirm these
findings. This article shows that a group randomly
assigned to 5 days of meditation practice with the integrative body–mind
training method shows significantly better
attention and control of stress than a similarly chosen control group
given relaxation
training. The training method comes from
traditional Chinese medicine and incorporates aspects of other
meditation and mindfulness
training. Compared with the control group, the
experimental group of 40 undergraduate Chinese students given 5 days of
20-min
integrative training showed greater improvement in
conflict scores on the Attention Network Test, lower anxiety,
depression,
anger, and fatigue, and higher vigor on the Profile
of Mood States scale, a significant decrease in stress-related
cortisol,
and an increase in immunoreactivity. These results
provide a convenient method for studying the influence of meditation
training
by using experimental and control methods similar
to those used to test drugs or other interventions.
Abstract
In recent years, a number of articles have demonstrated the benefits of various forms of meditation and mindfulness training
(1–8).
Many of them have tested practitioners of different meditative
practices and compared them with controls without any training.
Others have compared groups that were equivalent in
performance before training but had chosen whether or not to undertake
training (2, 4, 9, 10).
In a well done study (2), the experimental group received 3 months of training. The control group consisted of matched persons wanting to learn about
meditation. That study used a strong assay of attention, the attentional blink (11);
the two groups performed equally before meditation. The experimental
group was significantly better after meditation, suggesting
that meditation caused an improvement in the executive
attention network, most probably in the form of attention used by this
task (2).
Nevertheless, problems of subtle differences
between control and experimental groups remain in the study discussed
above,
because the two groups were not completely randomly
assigned, and meditators differed greatly in styles of meditation
previously
practiced (2). Slagter et al. (2)
said that “the absence of an association between the amount of prior
meditation training and our study results may be due
to the fact that there was great variation across the
practitioners in the styles and traditions of the previously learned
meditation. Longitudinal research examining and
comparing the effects of different styles of meditation on brain and
mental
function and the duration of such effects is needed.”
Styles of meditation differ. Some techniques such as concentration meditation, mantra, mindfulness meditation, etc. rely on
mind control or thought work, including focus on an object, paying attention to the present moment, etc. (2, 12–15). Mental training methods also share several key components, such as body relaxation, breathing practice, mental imagery,
and mindfulness, etc., which can help and accelerate practitioner access to meditative states (3, 8, 16–19).
This background raises the possibility that combining several key
components of body and mind techniques with features
of meditation and mindfulness traditions, while
reducing reliance on control of thoughts, may be easier to teach to
novices
because they would not have to struggle so hard to
control their thoughts. Therefore, integrative body–mind training (IBMT;
or simply integrative meditation) was developed in the
1990s, and its effects have been studied in China since 1995. Based
on the results from hundreds of adults and children
ranging from 4 to 90 years old in China, IBMT practice improves
emotional
and cognitive performance and social behavior (20, 21).
IBMT achieves the desired state by first
giving a brief instructional period on the method (we call it initial
mind setting
and its goal is to induce a cognitive or emotional set
that will influence the training). The method stresses no effort to
control thoughts, but instead a state of restful
alertness that allows a high degree of awareness of body, breathing, and
external instructions from a compact disc. It stresses
a balanced state of relaxation while focusing attention. Thought
control
is achieved gradually through posture and relaxation,
body–mind harmony, and balance with the help of the coach rather than
by making the trainee attempt an internal struggle to
control thoughts in accordance with instruction.
Training in this method is followed by 5 days
of group practice, during which a coach answers questions and observes
facial
and body cues to identify those people who are
struggling with the method. The trainees concentrate on achieving a
balanced
state of mind while being guided by the coach and the
compact disc that teaches them to relax, adjust their breathing, and
use mental imagery. Because this approach is suitable
for novices, we hypothesized that a short period of training and
practice
might influence the efficiency of the executive
attention network related to self-regulation (22).
In the present study, we used a random
assignment of 40 Chinese undergraduates to an experimental group and 40
to a control
group for 5 days of training 20 min per day. The
experimental group was given a short term of IMBT (module one) (20, 21).
Training was presented in a standardized way by compact disc and guided
by a skillful IBMT coach. Because of their importance
the coaches generally have several years of experience
with IBMT. The control group was given a form of relaxation training
very popular in the West (17, 23).
The two groups were given a battery of tests
1 week before training and immediately after the final training
session. A standard
computerized attention test measured orienting,
alerting, and the ability to resolve conflict (executive attention). The
Attention
Network Test (ANT) involves responding to an arrow
target that is surrounded by flankers that point either in the same or
opposite direction. Cues provide information on when a
trial will occur and where the target will be (24). The Raven's Standard Progressive Matrix (25, 26), which is a standard culture fair intelligence test; an assay of mood state, the Profile of Mood States (POMS) (27, 28); and a stress challenge of a mental arithmetic task were followed by measures of cortisol and secretory IgA (sIgA) (29–34). All of these are standard assays scored objectively by people blind to the experimental condition. See details in Materials and Methods.
In prior work (24), ANT has been used to measure skill in the resolution of mental conflict induced by competing stimuli. It activates a frontal
brain network involving the anterior cingulate gyrus and lateral prefrontal cortex (35, 36). Our underlying theory was that IBMT should improve functioning of this executive attention network, which has been linked
to better regulation of cognition and emotion (22, 37). Specifically, we hypothesized that (i) because assignment was random, the training and control group should not differ before training, (ii) the training would improve executive attention (indexed by ANT and POMS scales related to self-regulation and Raven's Standard
Progressive Matrices) in the experimental group more than for controls, and (iii)
if self-regulation ability improved in the training group, members of
the group should also show reduced reaction to stress
as measured by cortisol and immunoreactivity assays.
In short, the experimental group would show greater improvement in the
executive attention network related to
self-regulation; the Raven's intelligence test, which is known to differ
with improved
executive attention; mood scales related to
self-control; and cortisol and immunoreactivity measures of stress to a
mental
arithmetic challenge.
Previous SectionNext Section
Results
ANOVAs were conducted with group (trained
and control) and training session (before and after) as factors, using
each of the
attention network scores as dependent variables.
Before training, no differences were found for alerting, orienting, and
executive
networks in two groups (P > 0.05). The main effect of the training session was significant only for the executive network [F(1,78) = 9.859; P < 0.01]. More importantly, the group × session interaction was significant for the executive network [F(1,78) = 10.839; P
< 0.01], indicating that the before vs. after difference in the
conflict resolution score was significant only for the trained
group (see Fig. 1). The groups did not differ in orienting or alerting after training (P > 0.05). The result demonstrated that the short-term IBMT practice can influence the efficiency of executive attention.
Fig. 1.
Performance of
the ANT after 5 days of IBMT or control. Error bars indicate 1 SD.
Vertical axis indicates the difference in
mean reaction time between the congruent and
incongruent flankers. The higher scores show less efficient resolution
of conflict.
Because the efficiency of executive
attention improved, we expected better self-regulation of emotion. We
used the POMS to
measure emotion in the same two groups. Before
training, none of the six scales of POMS showed differences between the
two
groups (P > 0.05). The ANOVAs revealed a group × session effect for anger–hostility (A) [F(1,78) = 5.558; P < 0.05], depression–dejection (D) [F(1,78) = 5.558; P < 0.05], tension–anxiety (T) [F(1,78) = 11.920; P < 0.01], and vigor–activity (V) [F(1,78) = 7.479; P < 0.01]. After training, the t test indicated there were significant differences in the experimental group (but not the control group) in A, D, T, V, and
F (fatigue–inertia); in general, P (positive mood)average < 0.01 (see Fig. 2). The result indicated that short-term IBMT can enhance positive moods and reduce negative ones.
Fig. 2.
Comparison of
six scales of the POMS before and after training for the experimental
group. Blue bar, five negative moods and
one positive mood pretraining; red bar, five
negative moods posttraining; green bar, one positive mood posttraining.
Significance
was found in POMS scales of anger–hostility
(A), depression–dejection (D), fatigue–inertia (F), tension–anxiety (T),
and vigor–activity
(V) posttraining in the experimental group.
No significant difference was found in POMS scale C
(confusion–bewilderment) posttraining.
∗∗, P
average < 0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SD.
In previous work, the network associated with executive attention has been related to intelligence (38, 39). We also tested the hypothesis that improvement of efficiency of executive attention accompanies higher intelligence scores.
Scores of the Raven's Matrices did not differ before training (P > 0.05). The ANOVAs revealed the main effect of the training session; the Raven's improvement was significant [F(1,46) = 10.171; P < 0.01], but the group × session interaction was not significant, although there was a trend in that direction [F(1,46) = 3.102; P = 0.085]. The t tests showed a significant improvement in the experimental group after training (P < 0.001), but no significant improvement for the control group (P > 0.05). The result revealed that short-term IBMT can improve the Raven's score in the experimental group, although only
marginally more so than in the control group.
Cortisol and sIgA are indexes of the amount of stress induced by a cognitive challenge (29–34). We applied 3 min of mental arithmetic as an acute stress after 5 days of IBMT or relaxation. Fig. 3 shows that at baseline before stress there was no significant difference between the groups (P > 0.05). This finding indicated no sensitivity for cortisol and sIgA reactions to stress under normal states as found by
others (32, 34). After the arithmetic challenge, both groups increased in cortisol activity, indicating that the mental arithmetic challenge
was stressful (30).
Then the experimental group received an additional 20 min of IBMT and
the control 20 min of relaxation training. An ANOVA
showed that the group (experimental vs. control) ×
session (baseline before stress vs. after additional training)
interaction
was significant [F(1,38) = 6.281; P < 0.01]. The experimental group had a significantly lowered cortisol response to the mental stress after training than did
the control group (see Fig. 3
A).
Fig. 3.
Physiological changes before and after stress. (A)
Comparison of cortisol concentration between the experimental group
(red bars) and control group (gray bars) in three different
stages after 5 days of training. ∗, P < 0.05; ∗∗, P < 0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SEM. More cortisol secretion indicates higher levels of stress. (B) Comparison of sIgA concentration between experimental group (red bars) and control group (blue bars) at three different
stages after 5 days of training. ∗∗, P < 0.01. Error bars indicate 1 SEM. Higher immunoreactivity indicates a better response to stress.
Similarly, there was no significant difference between groups in sIgA at baseline before stress (P > 0.05). However, the mental arithmetic task resulted in a increase of sIgA relative to the baseline as found by others (34). After training, ANOVA showed the group × session interaction was significant [F(1,38) = 10.060; P < 0.001], with the arithmetic challenge resulting in significantly greater sIgA for the experimental group than the control
group (see Fig. 3
B).
Five days of training reduced the stress response to the mental challenge especially after an additional 20 min of practice.
Previous SectionNext Section
Discussion
In the ANT and POMS, the experimental
group showed significantly greater improvement after 5 days of IBMT than
the relaxation
control group. Because we randomly assigned
subjects to experimental and control groups and used objective tests
with researchers
blind to the condition, we conclude that IBMT
improved attention and self-regulation more than the relaxation control.
The
reaction to a mental stress was also significantly
improved in the experimental group, which showed less cortisol and more
immunoreactivity than the control group after the
additional training. These outcomes after only 5 days of training open a
door for simple and effective investigation of
meditation effects. The IBMT provides a convenient method for studying
the
influence of meditation training by using
appropriate experimental and control methods similar to those used to
test drugs
or other interventions. Our findings further
indicate the potential of IBMT for stress management, body–mind health,
and improvement
in cognitive performance and self-regulation (20, 21).
Although no direct measures of brain changes were used in this study, some previous studies suggest that changes in brain
networks can occur. Thomas et al. (40) showed that, in rats, one short experience of acute exposure to psychosocial stress reduced both short- and long-term survival
of newborn hippocampal neurons. Similarly, the human brain is sensitive to short experience. Naccache et al. (41)
showed that the subliminal presentation of emotional words (<100 ms)
modulates the activity of the amygdala at a long latency
and triggers long-lasting cerebral processes (41). Brefczynski-Lewis et al. (42)
compared novices, who participated in meditation 1 h per day by using
three different techniques with expert meditators
who had 10,000–54,000 h of practice. Both groups
showed activation of a large overlapping network of attention-related
brain
regions (42). Another situation where a single session changed brain processes is described by Raz et al. (43).
Highly hypnotizable persons when given an instruction to view a word as
nonsense showed elimination of the Stroop interference
effect and also eliminated activity in the anterior
cingulate during conflict trials (43). It was also demonstrated that 5 days of attention training with a computer program improved the efficiency of the executive
attention networks for children (39). Taken together, we have reason to believe that 5 days of IBMT practice could change brain networks, leading to improvements
in attention, cognition, and emotion and reaction to stress.
Why does IBMT work after only a few days of practice whereas studies with other methods often require months? The following
are possible reasons.
First, IBMT integrates several key components of body and mind techniques including body relaxation (17), breathing adjustment (18), mental imagery (44, 45), and mindfulness training (12–15), which have shown broad positive effects in attention, emotions, and social behaviors in previous studies (1–8). This combination may amplify the training effect over the use of only one of these components.
Second, because everyone experiences mindfulness sometimes (12, 13, 19, 46, 47), a qualified coach can help each participant increase the amount of this experience and thus guarantee that each practice
session achieves a good result (20, 21).
For participants with months to years of meditation, there has been the
opportunity to make mistakes, correct them, and
gradually find the right way. For 5 days of
training, quality practice is needed for every session. Recent findings
indicate
that the amount of time participants spent
meditating each day, rather than total number of hours of meditative
practice over
their lifetime, affects performance on attentional
tasks (10).
Third, in one study a selected comfortable music background showed more effects than Mozart's music (21).
Having music on the compact disc integrates the practice instruction
and occupies the novice's wandering mind via continuous
sensory input, maintaining and facilitating the
mindful state. Many meditation training methods use audiotapes or
compact
discs to help beginners (13, 46, 48).
We do not know which of these features is
of greatest importance in obtaining change, but we believe that the
integration
of the various methods into one easy-to-use
training package may explain why IBMT is effective at such a low dose.
We also
regard the work of the trainer as critical. The
trainer needs to know how to interact with the trainees to obtain the
desired
state. Although the trainers are not present during
the training sessions, they observe the trainees over television and
help
them after the session with problems. The trainers
could well be a part of the effective ingredient of IBMT, and their role
requires additional research.
We have thus far assessed the utility of
IBMT with random assignment only for Chinese undergraduates. However, we
do not believe
that any meditation method is appropriate for every
person. In other work, similar, but more preliminary, effects have been
observed for Chinese children and adults of many
ages. Preliminary data from studies in the United States suggest wider
utility
of IBMT (20, 21). Some may argue that IBMT effects require a prior belief in the benefits of meditation that would be common in China. However,
belief in meditation and traditional medicine is not high among undergraduates in modern China (49).
Also a relaxation group used as a control failed to achieve significant
improvement. One potentially important difference
between Eastern and Western meditation studies is
that in China we used group training at the same time of day for each
session
over 5 days; most of the Western studies used
individual meditation at different times. Studies of group dynamics
indicate
that the group acts to facilitate outcomes (50),
and possible differences between individual and group administration of
mediation needs further study. Studies of perception,
language, mathematics, and psychopathology suggest
differences between Americans and Chinese within each of these domains
(51–54).
Thus culture-specific experiences may subtly shape cognition and direct
neural activity in precise ways. If differences
between Chinese and American studies of attention
training arise, further studies will be needed to determine the reasons.
In previous work, executive attention has been shown to be an important mechanism for self-regulation of cognition and emotion
(22, 37, 39).
The current results with the ANT indicate that IBMT improves
functioning of this executive attention network. Studies designed
to improve executive attention in young children
showed more adult-like scalp electrical recordings related to an
important
node of the executive attention network in the
anterior cingulate gyrus (22, 37, 39).
We expect that imaging studies with adults would show changes in the
activation or connectivity of this network after IBMT.
Such studies would help to determine the mechanisms
by which IBMT improves performance. They may also provide a good
objective
basis for comparison between training methods.
In summary, IBMT is an easy, effective
way for improvement in self-regulation in cognition, emotion, and social
behavior.
Our study is consistent with the idea that
attention, affective processes, and the quality of moment-to-moment
awareness are
flexible skills that can be trained (55, 56).
Previous SectionNext Section
Materials and Methods
Participants.
Eighty
healthy undergraduates at the Dalian University of Technology [44 males,
mean age (± SD) = 21.8 ± 0.55] without any
training experiences participated in this study.
They were randomly assigned to an experimental or control group
(40:40).
Forty experimental subjects continuously
attended IBMT for 5 days with 20 min of training per day. Forty control
subjects
were given the same number and length of group
sessions but received information from the compact disc about relaxation
of
each body part. The coach observed them from a
closed-circuit television and provided answers to any question after
each training
session. The human experiment was approved by a
local Institutional Review Board, and informed consent was obtained from
each
participant. ANOVA and t tests were applied for analysis.
Measures.
The ANT was administered before and after training (24). Each person received 248 trials during each assessment session. Subtraction of reaction times was used to obtain scores
for each attentional network, including alerting, orienting, and conflict resolution (24).
The same two groups of 80 subjects took POMS (27, 28), and 48 subjects [28 males, mean age (± SD) = 21.7 ± 0.53] participated in the Raven's Matrices (25, 26) separately before and after 5 days of training.
Half of the experimental or control group
[20:20, 26 males, mean age (± SD) = 21.9 ± 0.97] was chosen randomly to
participate
in the physiological measures. Mental arithmetic
was used as an acute stressor after 5 days of IBMT and relaxation in two
groups (29–34).
Subjects were instructed to perform serial subtraction of 47 from a
four-digit number and respond verbally. During the
3-min mental arithmetic task, participants were
prompted to be as fast and accurate as possible. If the participants did
not
finish the mental arithmetic in time and correctly,
the computer would produce a harsh sound to remind the subjects, who
were
required to restart the task and do it again.
Cortisol and sIgA measures were taken
during three periods: baseline before stress, after mental stress, and
after additional
20-min training. First, all subjects were given a
5-min rest to get baseline. Second, all subjects were instructed to
finish
a 3-min mental arithmetic task to test whether
there were different stress reactions between two groups. Third, the
experimental
group practiced an additional 20-min IBMT, whereas
the control group relaxed for 20 min to test whether additional training
could improve the alterations based on 5 days of
training.
To control for variations of cortisol levels over the circadian rhythm, the math stress was performed from 2 p.m. to 6 p.m.
(32).
Saliva samples were collected immediately after each period by one-off
injectors and were encased in test tubes in succession,
with the tubes placed into a refrigerator under
−20°C and then thawed 24 h later for analysis. The concentration of
cortisol
and sIgA was analyzed by RIA at Dalian Medical
University (31–34).
IBMT Method.
IBMT involves several body–mind techniques including: (i) body relaxation, (ii) breath adjustment, (iii) mental imagery, and (iv) mindfulness training, accompanied with selected music background. In this study, IBMT module one was used. A compact disc
was developed for module one that included background music. IBMT module one practice included (i) presession, (ii) practice session, and (iii) postsession.
In the presession, usually 1 day before
the experiment, the coach gathered subjects to have a free
question-and-answer meeting
about IBMT practice via coaching techniques to
ensure the clear grasp of IBMT for the novices. The coach also set up
the exact
time, training room, and discipline for the group
practice. The most important thing for the coach was to create a
harmonious
and relaxed atmosphere for effective practice (20, 21).
In the training session, subjects
followed the compact disc with body posture adjustment, breathing
practice, guided imagery,
and mindfulness training accompanied by a music
background. Y.-Y.T. gave the practice instructions on the compact disc
himself.
The practice time was 20 min for 5 days. During the
training session, the coach observed facial and body cues to identify
those who were struggling with the method and gave
proper feedback immediately in postsession.
In the postsession, every subject filled out a questionnaire and evaluated the practice. The coach gave short responses to
subjects as required.
IBMT belongs to body–mind science in the
ancient Eastern tradition. Chinese tradition and culture is not only a
theory of
being but also (most importantly) a life experience
and practice. The IBMT method comes from traditional Chinese medicine,
but also uses the idea of human in harmony with
nature in Taoism and Confucianism, etc. The goal of IBMT is to serve as a
self-regulation practice for body–mind health and
balance and well being and to promote body–mind science research.
IBMT has three levels of training: (i) body–mind health, (ii) body–mind balance, and (iii)
body–mind purification for adults and one level of health and wisdom
for children. In each level, IBMT has theories and
several core techniques packaged in compact discs
or audiotapes that are instructed and guided by a qualified coach. A
person
who achieves the three levels of full training
after theoretical and practical tests can apply for instructor status.
IBMT involves learning that requires
experience and explicit instruction. To ensure appropriate experience,
coaches (qualified
instructors) are trained to help novices practice
IBMT properly. Instructors received training on how to interact with
experimental
and control groups to make sure they understand the
training program exactly. After each training session, the instructors
gave brief and immediate responses to questions
raised by the participants, helped those who were observed to be having
difficulties,
and asked each participant to fill out a
questionnaire and make any comments. The most important thing for
coaches was to
create a harmonious and relaxed atmosphere and give
proper feedback for effective practice. The coach believes everyone has
full potential and equality and that his/her job is
to find and enjoy a person's inner beauty and capacities to help them
think better and unfold their potentials rather
than to teach them.
A qualified coach is very important for
each level of teaching and practice. Without coaching, it is impossible
or very difficult
to practice IBMT with only compact discs.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Institute of
Neuroinformatics staff for assistance with data collection and Kewei
Chen and the RIA division at
Dalian Medical University for technical assistance.
This work was supported in part by National Natural Science Foundation
of China Grant 30670699, National 863 Plan Project
Grant 2006AA02Z431, Ministry of Education Grant NCET-06-0277, and a
Brain,
Biology, and Machine Initiative grant from the
University of Oregon.
Footnotes
-
¶To whom correspondence may be addressed. E-mail: yiyuan@uoregon.edu or mposner@uoregon.edu
-
Author contributions: J.W. and Y.F.
contributed equally to this work; Y.-Y.T., M.F., and M.I.P. designed
research; Y.-Y.T.,
Y.M., J.W., Y.F., S.F., Q.L., Q.Y., and D.S.
performed research; Y.-Y.T., Y.M., J.W., Y.F., S.F., Q.L., and Q.Y.
analyzed
data; and Y.-Y.T., M.K.R., and M.I.P. wrote
the paper.
-
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
- Abbreviations:
- IBMT,
- integrative body–mind training;
- ANT,
- Attention Network Test;
- POMS,
- Profile of Mood States;
- sIgA,
- secretory IgA.
- © 2007 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA
References
-
↵
- Lutz A ,
- Dunne JD ,
- Davidson RJ
(2007) in The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, eds
Zelazo
PD
,
Moscovitch
M
,
Thompson
E
(Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK) in press.
-
↵
- Slagter HA ,
- Lutz A ,
- Greischar LL ,
- Francis AD ,
- Nieuwenhuis S ,
- Davis JM ,
- Davidson RJ
(2007) PLoS Biol
5:e138.
-
↵
- Walsh R ,
- Shapiro SL
(2006) Am Psychol
61:227–239.
-
↵
- Davidson RJ ,
- Kabat-Zinn J ,
- Schumacher J ,
- Rosenkranz M ,
- Muller D ,
- Santorelli SF ,
- Urbanowski F ,
- Harrington A ,
- Bonus K ,
- Sheridan JF
(2003) Psychosom Med
65:564–570.
-
↵
- Baer R
(2003) Clin Psychol Sci Practice
10:125–143.
-
↵
- Grossman P ,
- Niemann L ,
- Schmidt S ,
- Walach H
(2004) J Psychosom Res
57:35–43.
-
↵
- Deshmukh VD
(2006) Sci World J
6:2239–2253.
-
↵
- Wallace BA ,
- Shapiro SL
(2006) Am Psychol
61:690–701.
-
↵
- Jain S ,
- Shapiro SL ,
- Swanick S ,
- Roesch SC ,
- Mills PJ ,
- Bell I ,
- Schwartz GE
(2007) Ann Behav Med
33:11–21.
-
↵
- Chan D ,
- Woollacott M
(2007) J Alt Complement Med, in press.
-
↵
- Shapiro KL ,
- Arnell KA ,
- Raymond JE
(1997) Trends Cognit Sci
1:291–296.
-
↵
- Hart W
(1987) The Art of Living: Vipassana Meditation: As Taught by SN Goenka (HarperOne, New York).
-
↵
- Kabat-Zinn J
(1994) Wherever You Go, There You Are: Mindfulness Meditation in Everyday Life (Hyperion, New York).
-
↵
- Langer EJ
(1990) Mindfulness (Addison–Wesley, Reading, MA).
-
↵
- Wallace BA
(2006) The Attention Revolution: Unlocking the Power of the Focused Mind (Wisdom, Somerville, MA).
-
↵
- Liu Y ,
- Vian K ,
- Eckman P ,
- Fang T ,
- Chen L
(1988) The Essential Book of Traditional Chinese Medicine (Columbia Univ Press, New York).
-
↵
- Benson H
(1975) The Relaxation Response (Avon, New York).
-
↵
- Lusk JT
(2005) Yoga Meditations (Whole Person Associates, Duluth, MN).
-
↵
- Suzuki S
(1973) Zen Mind, Beginner's Mind (Weatherhill, Boston, MA).
-
↵
- Tang Y
(2005) Health from Brain, Wisdom from Brain (Dalian University of Technology Press, Dalian, China).
-
↵
- Tang Y
(2007) Multi-Intelligence and Unfolding the Full Potentials of Brain (Dalian University of Technology Press, Dalian, China).
-
↵
- Posner MI ,
- Rothbart MK
(2007) Educating the Human Brain (APA Books, Washington, DC).
-
↵
- Bernstein DA ,
- Borkovec TD
(1973) Progressive Relaxation Training (Research Press, Champaign, IL).
-
↵
- Fan J ,
- McCandliss BD ,
- Sommer T ,
- Raz A ,
- Posner MI
(2002) J Cognit Neurosci
14:340–347.
-
↵
- Raven JC
(1938) Progressive Matrices: A Perceptual Test of Intelligence (HK Lewis, London).
-
↵
- Raven J ,
- Raven JC ,
- Court JH
(2003) Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales, Section 1: General Overview (Harcourt Assessment, San Antonio, TX).
-
↵
- Shacham S
(1983) J Personality Assessment
47:305–306.
-
↵
- Spinella M
(2007) J Gen Psychol
134:101–111.
-
↵
- Kirschbaum C ,
- Pirke KM ,
- Hellhammer DH
(1993) Neuropsychobiology
28:76–81.
-
↵
- Wang J ,
- Rao H ,
- Wetmore GS ,
- Furlan PM ,
- Korczykowski M ,
- Dinges DF ,
- Detre JA
(2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
102:17804–17809.
-
↵
- Polk DE ,
- Cohen S ,
- Doyle WJ ,
- Skoner DP ,
- Kirschbaum C
(2005) Psychoneuroendocrinology
30:261–272.
-
↵
- Hammerfald K ,
- Eberle C ,
- Grau M ,
- Kinsperger A ,
- Zimmermann A ,
- Ehlert U ,
- Gaab J
(2006) Psychoneuroendocrinology
31:333–339.
-
↵
- Deinzer R ,
- Kleineidam C ,
- Stiller-Winkler R ,
- Idel H ,
- Bachg D
(2000) Int J Psychophysiol
37:219–232.
-
↵
- Ring C ,
- Drayson M ,
- Walkey DG ,
- Dale S ,
- Carroll D
(2002) Biol Psychol
59:1–13.
-
↵
- Fan J ,
- McCandliss BD ,
- Fossella J ,
- Flombaum JI ,
- Posner MI
(2005) NeuroImage
26:471–479.
-
↵
- Posner MI ,
- Sheese BE ,
- Odludas Y ,
- Tang Y
(2006) Neural Netw
19:1422–1429.
-
↵
- Posner MI ,
- Rothbart MK ,
- Sheese BE ,
- Tang Y
(2007) Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci, in press.
-
↵
- Duncan J ,
- Seitz RJ ,
- Kolodny J ,
- Bor D ,
- Herzog H ,
- Ahmed A ,
- Newell FN ,
- Emslie H
(2000) Science
289:457–460.
-
↵
- Rueda MR ,
- Rothbart MK ,
- McCandliss BD ,
- Saccomanno L ,
- Posner MI
(2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
102:14931–14936.
-
↵
- Thomas RM ,
- Hotsenpiller G ,
- Peterson DA
(2007) J Neurosci
27:2734–2743.
-
↵
- Naccache L ,
- Gaillard RL ,
- Adam C ,
- Hasboun D ,
- Clemenceau S ,
- Baulac M ,
- Dehaene S ,
- Cohen L
(2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
102:7713–7717.
-
↵
- Brefczynski-Lewis JA ,
- Lutz A ,
- Schaefer HS ,
- Levinson DB ,
- Davidson RJ
(2007) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
104:11483–11488.
-
↵
- Raz A ,
- Fan J ,
- Posner MI
(2005) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
102:9978–9983.
-
↵
- McKinney CH ,
- Antoni MH ,
- Kumar M ,
- Tims FC ,
- McCabe PM
(1997) Health Psychol
16:390–400.
-
↵
- Watanabe E ,
- Fukuda S ,
- Shirakawa T
(2005) BMC Complementary Alt Med
5:21.
-
↵
- Goldstein J
(2003) Insight Meditation: The Practice of Freedom (Shambhala, Boston).
-
↵
- Lao T ,
- Mitchell S
(1988) Tao Te Ching (HarperCollins, New York).
-
↵
- Salzberg S ,
- Goldstein J
(2002) Insight Meditation: A Step-by Step Course on How to Mediatate (Sounds True, New York).
-
↵
- Zhang G
(2006) Med Philos (Humanistic Soc Med Ed)
27:14–17.
-
↵
- Heathfield SM
(2002) How to Build a Teamwork Culture: Do the Hard Stuff (Human Resources About.com, New York).
-
↵
- Siok WT ,
- Perfetti CA ,
- Jin Z ,
- Tan LH
(2004) Nature
431:71–76.
-
↵
- Tang Y ,
- Zhang W ,
- Chen K ,
- Feng S ,
- Ji Y ,
- Shen J ,
- Reiman EM ,
- Liu Y
(2006) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
103:10775–10780.
-
↵
- Nisbett RE ,
- Masuda T
(2003) Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
100:11163–11170.
-
↵
- Draguns JG ,
- Tanaka-Matsumi J
(2003) Behav Res Ther
41:755–776.
-
↵
- Tononi G ,
- Edelman GM
(1998) Science
282:1846–1851.
-
↵
- Posner MI ,
- DiGirolamo GJ ,
- Fernandez-Duque D
(1997) Consciousness Cognit
6:267–290.
Sorce: http://www.pnas.org/content/104/43/17152.long
|